老牛影视

Skip to main content

An Element of Surprise is the Recipe for Creating False Memories

Who鈥檚 to say, "No, it wasn鈥檛 like that?"

DURHAM, N.C. 鈥 You鈥檙e starting to tell that old story to a couple of new friends, and suddenly another person who was there says 鈥榥o, it wasn鈥檛 like that!鈥 Without a video recording to settle the dispute, it鈥檚 pretty hard to know who has the real memory and who has an adapted version.

Perhaps it鈥檚 no big deal to 鈥榤isremember鈥 like this in a social setting, but it鈥檚 quite another in a courtroom or classroom.

It turns out that human memory can be edited on the fly, creating memories that are nowhere near set in stone. A team of researchers has figured out how that happens and proved it by making people misremember.

鈥淚 think a lot of people have the misconception that memory works like a video camera: We record something and then we can play it back,鈥 said Allie Sinclair, a PhD candidate in Duke鈥檚 Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, who started the research as an undergraduate at the University of Toronto.

Alas, it鈥檚 not that simple or reliable. 鈥淲hen you recall a memory, your brain reconstructs that experience and sometimes, it edits the memory in the process,鈥 Sinclair said.

Memory editing is mostly a good thing, as it enables us to learn from our mistakes and integrate new information with old experiences. But the ability to edit leaves open the possibility of creating false memories as well.

For this study, which was conducted with colleagues at the University of Toronto, two dozen study participants were shown 70 short, unique video clips. 鈥淧eople genuinely seem to like this task, it鈥檚 like a movie-fest,鈥 Sinclair said.  

The following day, they were slid into the MRI tube to watch the videos again, but this time, half of the video clips were interrupted suddenly and without warning at the critical moment of the narrative, such as when the baseball batter was swinging at the pitch.

鈥淪urprise really engages your whole brain and activates some neuromodulatory systems, especially acetylcholine, dopamine, and norepinephrine,鈥 Sinclair said. 鈥淲hen something surprising happens, you're going to have a release of these neurotransmitters, and you're going to remember that event really strongly.鈥

On the third day, participants were interviewed in great detail to try to recall as much of the videos as they could. 鈥淎 couple of people were incredibly detailed and super accurate, but a couple of people had an insane number of false memories,鈥 Sinclair said. 鈥淚t was hard to keep a straight face.鈥

What the researchers saw in the MRI images is that surprise changed the role of the hippocampus, a brain region important for creating, retrieving, and editing memories. After unsurprising videos, the hippocampus seemed to be in 鈥減reserving mode,鈥 strengthening memories. But after surprising videos, the hippocampus switched into 鈥渦pdating mode,鈥 getting ready to edit memories. Surprise disrupted the stability of patterns in the hippocampus, showing this mode switch. More pattern disruption led to more false memories, and people were more likely to have false memories for the surprising videos that had been interrupted.

The rewriting of memories wasn鈥檛 entirely random however, Sinclair said. It seemed to happen between 鈥榮emantically related鈥 videos. For example, the baseball video might contaminate the memory of another sports-related clip.

鈥淲hen there are videos that are related to each other, that's where these new pieces of information are coming from,鈥 Sinclair said. 鈥淭here were occasional examples where somebody was clearly taking an element from one specific video and sticking it into another video.鈥

The finding throws more doubt onto the way crime witnesses recall events and faces -- showing them photos of things they haven鈥檛 witnessed is a bad idea -- but it also opens an opportunity to understand learning better, Sinclair said. Since surprise helps things stick, providing immediate feedback on wrong answers on a quiz would be helpful, as would the practice of asking students to try to predict an answer before providing them with the real one. 

鈥淚f we try to lead them to make active predictions, and then we give them surprising feedback, I think they would be more likely to learn from that feedback and really make it stick in memory,鈥 she said.

This research was supported by the James S. McDonnell Foundation, the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (RGPIN-2014-05959, RGPIN-2020-05747) and the U.S. National Science Foundation.

CITATION: 鈥淧rediction Errors Disrupt Hippocampal Representations and Update Episodic Memories,鈥 Alyssa Sinclair, Grace Manalili, Iva Brunec, R. Alison Adcock, Morgan Barense. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, Dec. 21, 2021. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.2117625118

ONLINE - https://www.pnas.org/content/118/51/e2117625118