A Hit of Dopamine Tells Baby Birds When Their Song Practice Is Paying Off

Brain scientists are beginning to disentangle the molecular mechanisms underlying the intrinsic motivation to learn.

Image
Skillful singing gives young zebra finches a dopamine hit. Credit: JJ Harrison

Watch a young male zebra finch learning to sing.

Thanks to new tools and techniques, including advances in machine learning and the ability to track subtle and rapid chemical fluctuations in the brain, Mooney and Duke neurobiology professor are beginning to disentangle the molecular signals that drive learning for its own sake.

In new research March 12 in the journal Nature, the team put male juvenile zebra finches into individual soundproof rooms where they could practice their songs at will.

In zebra finches, only the males sing; young birds learn their courtship song early in life by first listening closely to their dad and memorizing his song. Then, like babies learning to talk, they begin to babble, their squeaks slowly becoming more song-like. By practicing their songs and listening to the results, gradually they figure out how to produce the right notes and rhythms to match their mental template of their dad鈥檚 song.

It takes a zebra finch chick about three months from hatching to become a proficient singer.

To Mooney, a longtime rock 'n' roll fan, the males鈥 practice sessions are a bit like the for The Beatles. 鈥淭he Beatles might have done a hundred takes鈥 before they were satisfied, Mooney said. Similarly, 鈥渢hese birdsong data sets get so big so fast.鈥

Study authors Richard Mooney, Jiaxuan Qi and John Pearson

That's where Pearson鈥檚 team came in. To get a handle on the data, they developed a machine learning model that can analyze the thousands of song renditions and score each attempt.

鈥淭his way we can track learning on a moment-by-moment basis,鈥 Pearson said.

鈥淪ome tries were a little better, and some were a little worse,鈥 he added. Generally the longer the birds worked at it, the better they got.

As the birds gradually mastered their tunes, the team also measured the level of dopamine released in the birds鈥 basal ganglia, a part of the brain involved in learning new motor skills.

Dopamine is one of the brain鈥檚 chemical messengers, transmitting important signals about learning, reward and motivation from one neuron to another.

To monitor dopamine, the researchers used tiny sensors made from genetically modified proteins that glow when particular neurochemicals are released in the brain. The technology makes it possible to track brain activity that is largely invisible to common methods based on measuring electrical signals.

What they found surprised them. Whenever a bird practiced, dopamine levels in the bird鈥檚 basal ganglia started to ramp up. It didn鈥檛 matter whether they hit all the notes or missed the mark. In other words, any effort at singing activates signals in the brain鈥檚 reward pathways.

The birds鈥 dopamine surged more when a bird performed better than was typical for their age. The signal was slightly weaker when they regressed.

The better they performed for their age, the more dopamine increased, said first author Jiaxuan Qi, who did the work as in neurobiology at Duke.

Dopamine has long been known to play an important role in how humans and other animals learn from external rewards and punishments.

Take, for example, a child studying because they want to get a good grade or avoid a scolding. Or consider a rat learning to press a lever for food.

But birds don鈥檛 need carrots or sticks to learn how to sing. Because the birds were alone during their practice sessions, singing away in a soundproof room, they weren鈥檛 getting any external feedback on how they were doing.

鈥淣obody's telling the bird if he鈥檚 an honor student or going to be sent to detention,鈥 Mooney said.

Instead, the findings suggest that dopamine acts like an internal 鈥渃ompass鈥 to steer their learning.

The team鈥檚 research helps explain how learning still occurs even in the absence of external incentives. The researchers also found that dopamine isn鈥檛 the only chemical signal required for such learning.

Qi was able to show that another chemical messenger called acetylcholine can trigger dopamine release in the bird鈥檚 brain when it is singing. It works by binding to a different part of the neurons, giving the bird an extra dopamine boost when it belts its ballad.

When the birds were given drugs that blocked dopamine or acetylcholine signaling in the basal ganglia, the birds made less progress, Qi added.

鈥淟earning basically comes to a halt,鈥 Mooney said. 鈥淭he bird still sings a lot, but he doesn't seem to be able to learn from it.鈥

The potential implications go beyond bird brains, Mooney said.

鈥淭hese findings translate across species,鈥 Pearson said. 鈥淭he brain regions and neurochemicals involved -- namely the basal ganglia, dopamine and acetylcholine -- are shared by mice, primates, humans. Essentially every animal with a backbone.鈥

Studying how birds learn to sing can help researchers better understand how humans learn other motor skills such as talking or juggling or playing an instrument. In that sense, Mooney said, 鈥渂irdsong learning is very similar to what children do when they spontaneously acquire these remarkable skills.鈥

Dopamine signaling problems in the basal ganglia have also been linked to a number of diseases, including Parkinson鈥檚 and schizophrenia.

鈥淚t's really important that we understand these regions, and the bird is a means of getting at those principles,鈥 Pearson said.

鈥淥f all the scientific frontiers that remain, the brain is probably the most poorly understood, and it's fundamental to being human,鈥 Mooney said.

This research was supported by grants from the National Institutes of Health (5R01 NS099288, RF1 NS118424, F32 MH132152 and F31 NS132469).

Citation

"Dual Neuromodulatory Dynamics Underlie Birdsong Learning," Jiaxuan Qi, Drew C. Schreiner, Miles Martinez, John Pearson & Richard Mooney. Nature, March 12, 2025. DOI: 10.1038/s41586-025-08694-9