Even Preschoolers Can Spot a Cop-Out
When it comes to broken promises, children as young as 3 to 5 recognize that some excuses are better than others.
In a published in the journal Cognitive Development, 64 3- and 5-year-olds were shown a series of videos in which puppets promised to show them a cool toy, left the scene to go get it, but then came back empty-handed.
Afterwards, the puppets either gave a good excuse for going back on their word (鈥淚 had to help my friend with his homework鈥), a bad excuse (鈥淚 wanted to watch TV鈥), or no explanation at all. The children were then asked whether they thought the puppets鈥 actions were wrong or not, and why.
No matter what the excuse (or lack thereof), the children agreed that it was generally wrong to break a promise. But they were more understanding when the puppets offered a good excuse (i.e., they had to help someone), versus a lame one (i.e., they just wanted to do something fun instead).
In other words, children this age grasp that obligations to help others take priority over selfish desires, Li said.
The children鈥檚 responses also revealed that a lame excuse was just as bad as none at all.
鈥淧revious research has suggested that in some cases, young kids will just take any reason to be better than no reason at all,鈥 Li said. 鈥淏ut here we showed that kids do pay attention to the actual content.鈥
When asked to explain their answers, the children鈥檚 justifications changed with age. Compared with 3-year-olds, 5-year-olds were better at articulating their thinking in terms of what the puppets 鈥渟hould鈥 do, or are 鈥渟upposed鈥 to do, suggesting that kids鈥 understanding of obligations to others is more fully developed by this age.
Surprisingly, lousy excuses didn鈥檛 make the children less inclined to say they 鈥渓iked鈥 some puppets, or would invite those puppets to a play date.
鈥淯sually if someone breaks a promise and gives you a lame reason, it implies they're not really a good friend,鈥 Li said. 鈥淐hildren this age don鈥檛 make that connection. They're just not there yet.鈥
This study is part of a larger field of research on how children come to appreciate and act on cultural and moral norms for how we behave and treat each other.
鈥淢orality is a type of common ground that we have with others, with mutual expectations about how we should behave and what counts as good grounds for justification,鈥 Li said.
鈥淲e're showing that young children become attuned to this common ground at an early age.鈥
Li said the findings are also relevant to any adult who has uttered the classic fallback phrase, 鈥淏ecause I said so.鈥
鈥淜ids are paying attention and can tell that is a lame reason,鈥 Li said.
This research was supported by the Duke Department of Psychology and Neuroscience.
Citation
CITATION: "Young Children Judge Defection Less Negatively When There鈥檚 a Good Justification," Leon Li, Aren Tucker, Michael Tomasello. Cognitive Development, Nov. 3, 2022. DOI: